Literaturverzeichnis

In: Inhibition
Free access

Literaturverzeichnis

Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ainslie, G. (2001). Breakdown of Will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Allport, A. (1987). Selection for Action: Some Behavioral and Neurophysiological Considerations of Attention and Action. In Perspectives on Perception and Action, Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: 395419.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Alvarez, M. (2013). Agency and Two‐Way Powers. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 113 (1): 101121.

Amaya, S. (2013). Slips. Noûs, 47 (3): 559576.

Anscombe, G.E.M. (1957). Intention. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Aristotle (2004). The Nicomachean Ethics, übersetzt von J.A.K. Thompson. London: Penguin Press.

Aron, A.R., Robbins, T.W., Poldrack, R.A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8 (4): 170177.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Aron, A.R., Poldrack, R.A. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57 (11): 12851292.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Aron, A.R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist, 13 (3): 214228.

Aron A.R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69 (12): 5568.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Aron, A.R., Robbins, T.W. Poldrack, R.A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4): 177185.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Austin J.L. (1961). Ifs and Cans. In J.L. Austin: Philosophical Papers, herausgegeben von J.O. Urmson und G.J. Warnock. Oxford: Clarendon Press: 205232.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Balestra, C., Duchateau, J., Hainaut, K. (1992). Effects of fatigue on the stretch reflex in a human muscle. Electroencephalography Clinical Neurophysiology, 85(1): 4652.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Banich M.T., Compton, R.J. (2011). Cognitive Neuroscience. Wadworth: Cengage Learning.

Bargh, J.A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R.S. Wyer, Jr. und T.K. Srull (Hrsg.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic processes; Applications. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc: 140.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Bargh, J.A. (1997). The Automaticity of Everyday Life. In R.S. Wyer Jr., (Hrsg.). Advances in Social Cognition, Vol. 10. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: 161.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Bargh, J.A., Chartrand, T.L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54 (7): 462479.

Bari, A., Robbins, T.W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108: 4479.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Batterink L, Yokum S, Stice E. (2010). Body Mass correlates inversely with Inhibitory Control in response to Food among adolescent Girls: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 52: 16961703.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Baumeister, R.F., Heatherton, T.F., Tice, D.M. (1994). Losing Control. How and why People fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., Tice, D. (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the Active Self a Limited Resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5): 12521265.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Baumeister, R.F. (2002). Yielding to Temptation: Self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behaviour. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4): 670676.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Baumeister, R.F., Vohs, K.D., Tice, M.D. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16: 351355.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Baumeister, R.F., Tienery, J. (2011). Willpower. Why Self-Control is the Secret to Success. London: Penguin Press.

Békésy von, G. (1967). Sensory Inhibition. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Berridge, K.C., Kringelbach, M.L. (2015). Pleasure Systems in the Brain, Neuron, 86(3): 646664.

Biran, I., Giovannetti, T., Buxbaum, L., Chatterjee, A. (2006). The alien hand syndrome: What makes the alien hand alien? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(4): 563582.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Blakemore, R.L., Vuilleumier, P. (2017) An Emotional Call to Action: Integrating Affective Neuroscience in Models of Motor Control. Emotion Review, 9 (4): 299309.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Brass M., Haggard P. (2007). To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(34): 91419145.

Brass M., Haggard P. (2008). The What, When, Whether Model of Intentional Action. Neuroscientist, 14 (4): 319-325.

Bratman, M.E. (1987). Intention, plans, and practical reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bratman, M.E. (2007). Structures of Agency: Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Brozzo, C. (2017). Motor intentions: How intentions and motor representations come together. Mind and Language 32(2): 231256.

Buchsbaum, B.R., Greer, S., Chang, W.L., Berman, K.F. (2005). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin card-sorting task and component processes. Human Brain Mapping, 25: 3545.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Buehler, D. (2018). The central executive system. Synthese, 195 (5): 19691991.

Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Butterfill, S., Sinigaglia, C. (2014). Intention and Motor Representation in Purposive Action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 88: 119145.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Campbell, J. (2002). Reference and Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cheng, P.W., Novick, L.R. (1991). Causes versus Enabling Conditions. Cognition, 40: 83120.

Chisholm, R. (1966). Freedom and Action. In K. Lehrer (Hrsg.), Freedom and Determinism. New Work Random House: 1144.

Cisek, P. (2006). Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: A computational model. Journal of Neuroscience, 26 (38): 97619770.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Cisek, P. (2007). Cortical mechanisms of action selection: The affordance competition hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 362: 15851599.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Cisek, P., Kalaska, J.F. (2010). Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33: 269298.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Clarke, R. (2003). Libertarian Accounts of Free Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Clarke, R. (2010). Skilled Activity and the Causal Theory of Action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 80 (3): 523550.

Cooper, R., Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17 (4): 297338.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Cooper, R., Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical Schemas and Goals in the Control of Sequential Behavior. Psychological Review, 113 (4): 887916.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Cracco, E., Genschow, O., Radkova, I., Brass, M. (2017). Automatic imitation of pro- and antisocial gestures: Is implicit social behavior censored? Cognition, 170: 179189.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Cracco, E., Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Genschow, O., Rigoni, D., De Coster, L., Radkova, I., Deschrijver, E., Brass, M. (2018). Automatic imitation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114 (5): 453500.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. (1994). Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language. Cambridge MA: Academic Press.

Dalrymple-Alford, E.C. Budayer, B. (1966). Examination of some aspects of the Stroop Color-Word Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23(3): 12111214.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Davidson, D. (1963). Actions, reasons, and causes. Journal of Philosophy, 60 (23): 685700.

Davidson, D. (1980). Essays on Actions and Events. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Davidson, D. (1980). Freedom to Act. In D. Davidson (Hrsg.). Essays on Actions and Events. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 6382.

Davies, M. (2000). Persons and their underpinnings. Philosophical Explorations, 3: 4362.

Della Sala S., Marchetti C., Spinnler, H. (1991). Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: A longitudinal study. Neuropsychologia, 29(11): 11131127.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dempster, F.N., Brainerd, C.J. (1995). Interference and Inhibition in Cognition. Cambridge MA: Academic Press.

Dempster, F.N. (1995). Interference and inhibition in cognition: an historical perspective. In F.N. Dempster und C.J. Brainerd (Hrsg.). Interference and inhibition in cognition. New York: Academic Press: 326.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dennett, D. (1969). Content and Consciousness. New York: Humanities Press.

Dennett, D. (1978). Toward a Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. In D. Dennett (Hrsg.) Brainstorms. Hassocks: Harvester: 149173.

Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.

Descartes, R. (1649). Passions of the Soul. In The Passions of the Soul and Other Late Philosophical Writings übersetzt von M. Moriarty. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2015.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Descartes, R. (1664). Traitée de L’Homme. Paris: Girard.

Desimone R., Duncan J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review Neuroscience, 18: 193222.

Dewey, J. (1922). Human Nature and Conduct. New York: Henry Holt and Co.

Diamond, S., Balvin, R.S., Diamond, F.R. (Hrsg.) (1963). Inhibition and Choice: A Neurobiological Approach to Problems of Plasticity of Behaviour. New York: Harpen and Row.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dillon, G.D., Pizzagalli, D.A. (2007). Inhibition of Action, Thought and Emotion: A Selective Neurobiological Review, Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3): 99114.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Donders, F.C. (1862). Über die Schnelligkeit psychischer Processe. Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie: 657-681.

Dretske, F. (1988). Explaining Behavior: Reasons in a World of Causes. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Dretske, F. (1993). Mental events as structuring causes of behavior. In J. Heil und A.R. Mele (Hrsg.), Mental Causation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dretske, F. (2010). Triggering and Structuring Causes. In T. O’Connor und C. Sandis (Hrsg.): A Companion to the Philosophy of Action. Hoboken NJ: Wiley-Blackwell: 139144.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dreyfus, H. (1993). Heidegger’s Critique of the Husserl/Searle Account of Intentionality. In H.L. Dreyfus (2014): Skillful Coping. Essays on the Phenomenology of Everyday Perception and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 7691.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Dreyfus, H. (2001) The Primacy of Phenomenology over Logical Analysis. In H.L. Dreyfus (2014): Skillful Coping. Essays on the Phenomenology of Everyday Perception and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 146167.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Engber, D. (2016). Everything is Crumbling. Slate. Online: http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/cover_story/2016/03/ego_depletion_an_influential_theory_in_psychology_may_have_just_been_debunked.html?via=gdpr-consent. (letzter Aufruf 28. August 2018)

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Enç, B. (2003). How We Act: Causes, Reasons, and Intentions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49(8): 709724.

Evans J.S. (2008). Dual-Processing Accounts of Reasoning, Judgment, and Social Cognition Annual Review of Psychology, 59: 255278.

Evans, J.S., Frankish K. (2009). In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 3154.

Field, M., Cox, W.M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, Volume 97 (1-2): 120.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ferrier, D. (1876). The Functions of the Brain. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons.

Ferrier, D. (1886). The Functions of the Brain, zweite überarbeitete Auflage New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons.

Filevich, E., Kühn, S., Haggard, P. (2012). Intentional inhibition in human action: The power of »no«. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 36(4): 11071118.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ford, A., Hornsby J., Stoutland, F. (2011). Essays on Anscombe’s Intention. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Frankfurt, H.G. (1971). Freedom of the Will and the Concept of a Person. Journal of Philosophy, 68 (1): 520.

Frankfurt, H.G. (1978). The Problem of Action. American Philosophical Quarterly, 15 (2): 157162.

Frankfurt, H.G. (1982). The Importance of What We Care About. Synthese, 53 (2): 257272.

Frankfurt, H.G. (1988). Identification and Wholeheartedness. In H.G. Frankfurt: The Importance of What We Care About. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 159176.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1910). Die psychogene Sehstörung in psychoanalytischer Auffassung. In S. Freud: Studienausgabe Bd. VI. Hysterie und Angst. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 1971: 205214.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1915a). Verdrängung. In S. Freud: Das Ich und das Es. Metapsychologische Schriften. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 2010: 103124.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1915b). Das Unbewusste. In S. Freud: Das Ich und das Es. Metapsychologische Schriften. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 2010: 117153.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1917). Vorlesungen zur Einführung in die Psychoanalyse. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 2010.

Freud, S. (1923). Das Ich und das Es. In S. Freud: Das Ich und das Es. Metapsychologische Schriften. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 2010: 251295.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1926). Hemmung, Symptom und Angst. In S. Freud: Werke aus den Jahren 1925-1931. Frankfurt a. Main: S. Fischer Verlag, 1968: 112205.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Freud, S. (1901/1960). The Psychopathology of Everyday Life. London: Hogarth Press.

Frost, K. (2013). Action as the Exercise of a Two-Way Power. Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 56 (6): 611624.

Frost, K. (2020). What Could a Two-Way Power Be? Topoi, 39 (5): 11411153.

Gailliot, M., Baumeister, R., DeWall, N., Maner, J. (2007). Self-Control Relies on Glucose as a Limited Energy Source: Willpower Is More Than a Metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2): 325336.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ganos, C., Kühn, S., Kahl, U., Schunke, O., Feldheim, J., Gerloff, C., Roessner, V., Bäumer, T., Thomalla, G., Haggard, P., Münchau, A. (2014). Action inhibition in Tourette syndrome. Movement Disorders, 29 (12): 15321538.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ganos, C., Asmuss, L., Bongert, J., Brandt V., Münchau, A., Haggard, P. (2015). Volitional action as perceptual detection: predictors of conscious intention in adolescents with tic disorders. Cortex, 64: 4754.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Gibson, J.J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Ginet, C. (1990). On Action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Giovannetti T, Buxbaum LJ, Biran I, Chatterjee A. (2005). Reduced endogenous control in alien hand syndrome: evidence from naturalistic action. Neuropsychologia, 43(1): 7588.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Glimcher P.W. (2003). The neurobiology of visual-saccadic decision-making. Annual Review Neuroscience, 26: 133179.

Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54: 493503.

Grèzes, J., Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40(2): 212222.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armony, J., Ellis, R., Passingham, R.E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(12): 27352740.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Grice, P. (1974). Method in Philosophical Psychology (From the Banal to the Bizarre). Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, 48: 2353.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Gross, J.J., John, O.P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2): 348362.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hagenaars, M.A., Roelofs, K. Stins, J.F. (2014). Human freezing in response to affective films. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 27(1): 2737.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hagenaars, M.A., Oitzl, M., Roelofs, K. (2014). Updating freeze: Aligning animal and human research. Neuroscience and Biehavioural Reviews, 47: 165176.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hager, M.S et al. (2016). A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4): 546573.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Haidt J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4): 814834.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hall, P.A. (2012). Executive control resources and frequency of fatty food consumption: Findings from an age-stratified community sample. Health Psychology, 31(2), 235241.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Harnishfeger, K.K. (1995). The development of cognitive inhibition. In F.N. Dempster und C.J. Brainerd (Hrsg.). Interference and inhibition in cognition. NewYork: Academic Press: 175204.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hart. H.L., Honoré. A.M. (1959). Causation in the Law. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Helmholtz, H. (1852). Messungen über die Fortplanzungsgeschwindigkeit der Reizung der Nerven. Archiv für Anatomie, Physiologie und wissenschaftliche Medizin: 199216.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Henden, E. (2008). What is self-control? Philosophical Psychology, 21 (1): 6990.

Hendrick, O.M., Luo X., Zhang, S. Li C.S. (2012). Saliency processing and obesity: preliminary imaging study of the stop signal task. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2012, 20 (9): 17961802.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Henning, T. (2016). Gegenwart. In M. Kühler und M. Rüther (Hrsg.): Handbuch Handlungstheorie. Grundlagen, Kontexte, Perspektive. Stuttgart: Metzler Verlag: 4557.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hill, J.O., Peters, J.C. (1998). Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science, 280: 13711374.

Hochstetter, G. (2016). Attention in bodily awareness. Synthese, 193 (12): 38193842.

Hochstetter, G., Wong, H.Y. (2017). Comment: Affective Control of Action. Emotion Review, 9 (4): 345348.

Hofmann, W., Baumeister R.F., Förster, G., Vohs, K.D. (2012). Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.102(6): 13181335.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and Self-Control From a Dual-Systems Perspective. Perspectives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2): 162176.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Roefs, A. (2009). Three ways to resist temptation: The independent contributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation on the impulse control of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45: 431435.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hofmann W., Baumeister R.F., Förster G., Vohs K.D. (2012). Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102: 13181335.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Holton, R. (2003). How is strength of will possible? In C. Tappolet und S. Stroud (Hrsg.), Weakness of Will and Practical Irrationality. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 3967.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Holton, R. (2009). Willing, Wanting, Waiting. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hommel, B. (2000). The Prepared Reflex: Automaticity and Control in Stimulus Response Translation, in S. Monsell und J.S. Driver (Hrsg.) Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press: 243273.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 24: 849937.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hornsby, J. (1980). Actions. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Hornsby, J. (2000). Personal and sub-personal. A defense of Dennett’s early distinction. Philosophical Explorations, 3: 624.

Hornsby, J. (2010). Trying to Act. In T. O’Connor und C. Sandis (Hrsg.): A Companion to the Philosophy of Action. London: Wiley-Blackwell: 1825.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Hornsby, J. (2013). Basic Activity. Aristotelian Society Supplementary Volume, 87 (1): 118.

Hruby, A., Hu, F.B. (2015). The Epidemiology of Obesity: A Big Picture. Pharmacoeconomics, 33(7): 673689.

Hursthouse, R. (1991). Arational actions. Journal of Philosophy, 88 (2): 5768.

Hyman, J. (2017). Action, Knowledge and Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B.J. (2012). What Is Ego Depletion? Toward a Mechanistic Revision of the Resource Model of Self-Control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5):450463.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. Two Volumes. New York: Henry Holt and Co.

Jónsdóttir, M., Adólfsdóttir, S., Cortez, R.D., Gunnarsdóttir, M., Gústafsdóttir, A.H. (2007). A diary study of action slips in healthy individuals. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21 (6): 875883.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58 (9): 697720.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Kalanthroff, E., Cohen, N., Henik, A. (2013). Stop feeling: Inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 17.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kalaska, J.F., Rizzolatti, G. (2013). Voluntary Movements: The Primary Motor Cortex. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, T.M. Jessell, S.A. Siegelbaum und A.J. Hudespeth (Hrsg.) Principles of Neural Science. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies: 834864.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kane, R. (1996). The Significance of Free Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Karniol, R., Miller, D.T. (1983). Why not wait?: A cognitive model of self-imposed delay termination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 935942.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kenner, N.M., Mumford, J.A., Hommer, R.E., Skup, M., Leibenluft, E., Poldrack, R.A. (2010). Inhibitory motor control in response stopping and response switching. In: Journal of Neuroscience, 30(25): 85128518.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kennett, J., Smith, M. (1996). Frog and toad lose control. Analysis, 56: 6373.

Kennett, J., Smith, M. (1997). Synchronic self-control is always non-actional. Analysis, 57: 123131.

Kenny, A. (1975). Will, Freedom and Power. Oxford: Blackwell.

Koningsbruggen van, G.M., Stroebe, W., Papies, E.K., Aarts, H. (2011). Implementation intentions as goal primes: Boosting self-control in tempting environments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41: 551557.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kritikos A., Breen N., Mattingley J.B. (2005). Anarchic hand syndrome: bimanual coordination and sensitivity to irrelevant information in unimanual reaches. Cognitive Brain Research, 24 (3): 634-647.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Kühn, S., Haggard, P., Brass, M. (2009). Intentional inhibition: how the veto-area exerts control. Human Brain Mapping, 30 (9), 28342843.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Lawrence, N.S., O’Sullivan, J., Parslow, D., Javaid, M., Adams, R.C., Chambers, C.D., Kos, K., Verbruggen, F. (2015). Training response inhibition to food is associated with weight loss and reduced energy intake. Appetite, 95: 1728.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Lazarus, R.S., Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive appraisal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69: 195205.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Levy, N. (2011). Resisting ›Weakness of Will‹. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 82 (1): 134155.

Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilization behavior and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106: 237255.

Libet, B.W. (1999). Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6 (8-9): 4757.

Logan, G.D., Schachar, R.J., Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8 (1), 6064.

Mackie. J.L. (1965). Causes and conditions. American Philosophical Quarterly, 2(4): 245264.

MacLeod, C.M., (2007). The concept of inhibition in cognition. In D.S. Gorfein und C.M. MacLeod (Hrsg.). Inhibition in cognition. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association: 323.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Maier, J. (2018). »Abilities«, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2018 Edition), E.N. Zalta (Hrsg.), URL = <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2018/entries/abilities/>.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mayr, E. (2011). Understanding Human Agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

McBride J., Sumner, P., Jackson S.R., Bajaj N., Husain M. (2013). Exaggerated object affordance and absent automatic inhibition in alien hand syndrome. In Cortex, 49 (8): 20402054.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

McCann, H. (1986). Intrinsic Intentionality. Theory and Decision, 20: 247273.

McDougall, W. (1903). The nature of inhibitory processes within the nervous system. Brain 26: 153191.

McDowell, J. (1994). The Content of Perceptual Experience. Philosophical Quarterly, 44: 175205.

Meister M., Tessier-Lavigne, M. (2013). Low-level Visual Processing. The Retina. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, T.M. Jessell, S.A. Siegelbaum und A.J. Hudespeth (Hrsg.) Principles of Neural Science. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies: 577601.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mele, A.R. (1987). Irrationality: An Essay on ›Akrasia‹, Self-Deception, and Self-Control. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mele, A.R. (1992). Springs of Actions: Understanding intentional Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mele, A.R. (1995). Autonomous Agents: From Self-Control to Autonomy. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mele, A.R. (1997). Underestimating self-control: Kennett and Smith on frog and toad. Analysis, 57: 119123.

Mele, A.R. (1998). Synchronic self-control revisited: Frog and toad shape up. Analysis, 58, 305310.

Mele, A.R. (2003). Motivation and Agency. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mele, A.R. (2010). Motivational Strength. In T. O’Connor und C. Sandis (Hrsg.): A Companion to the Philosophy of Action. Hoboken NJ: Wiley-Blackwell: 259266.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mele, A.R. (2012). Backsliding. Understanding Weakness of Will. New York: Oxford University Press.

Metcalfe, J., Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 319.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Milyavskaya, M., Inzlicht, M. (2017). Attentional and Motivational Mechanisms of Self-Control. In D.T.D. de Ridder, M. Adriaanse, und K. Fujita (Hrsg.). Handbook of Self-Control in Health & Well- Being. Abingdon UK: Routledge: 1123.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mischel, W., Ebbeson, E.R. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16: 329337.

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., Rodriguez, M.L. (1992). Delay of Gratification in Children. In G. Lowenstein und J. Elster (Hrsg.): Choice Over Time. New York: Russell Sage Foundation: 147164.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mischel, W. (1996). From Good Intentions to Willpower. In P. Gollwitzer und J. Bargh (Hrsg.), The Psychology of Action. Linking cognition and motivation to behavior. New York: Guilford Press: 197218.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mole, C. (2011). Attention is Cognitive Unison: An Essay in Philosophical Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

Monsell, S. (1996). Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce (Hrsg.) Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition. Oxford: Erlbaum UK; Taylor & Francis; 93148.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Monsell, S., Driver, J. (2000). Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (3): 134140.

Moore, B.D., Drouin, J., Gansneder B.M., Shultz, S.J. (2002). The differential effects of fatigue on reflex response timing and amplitude in males and females. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 12(5): 351–3360.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Moors, A. (2009). Automaticity. In T. Bayne, A. Cleeremans, P. Wilken (Hrsg.), Oxford Companion to Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 9195.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Moors, A., De Houwer, J. (2007). What is automaticity? An analysis of its component features and their interrelations. In J.A. Bargh (Hrsg.). Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. Frontiers of Social Psychology. New York: Psychology Press: 1150.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Moran, R. (2004). Anscombe on Practical Knowledge. In: Philosophy, 55 (2004): 4368.

Morein-Zamir, S., Fineberg, N.A., Robbins, T.W., Sahakian, B.J. (2010). Inhibition of thoughts and actions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: extending the endophenotype? Psychological Medicine, 40: 263272.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Moro, V., Pernigo, S., Scandola, M., Mainente, M., Avesani, R., Agiolti, S.M. (2015). Contextual bottom-up and implicit top-down modulation of anarchic hand syndrome: A single-case report and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia, 78 (11): 122129.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Munakata, Y., Snyder, H.R., Chatham, C.H. (2012). Developing Cognitive Control: Three Key Transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2): 7177.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Muraven, M., Tice, D.M., Baumeister, R. (1998). Self-Control as a Limited Resource: Regulatory Depletion Patterns, Journal, of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (3): 774789.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Muraven, M., Baumeister, R., Tice, D.M. (1999). Longitudinal Improvement of Self- Regulation Through Practice: Building Self-Control Strength Through Repeated Exercise, The Journal of Social Psychology, 139 (4): 446457.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. (2000). Self-Regulation and Depletion of Limited Resources: Does Self-Control Resemble a Muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126 (2): 247259.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Mylopoulos, M., Pacherie, E. (2018). Intentions: The dynamic hierarchical model revisited. WIREs Cognitive Science: 113.

Nanay, B. (2013). Between Perception and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F., Havermans, R., Roefs, A., Jansen, A. (2006). Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite, 47: 253256.

Neill, W.T., Valdes, L.A., & Terry, K.M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In F.N. Dempster und C.J. Brainerd (Hrsg.). Interference and inhibition in cognition. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press: 207261.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Neumann, O. (1987) Beyond Capacity: A Functional View of Attention. In Perspectives on Perception and Action. Hillsdale N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: 361394.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Norman, D.A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88 (1): 115.

Norman, D.A., Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behaviour. In M. Lansman und E. Hunt (Hrsg.) Proceedings of the Lake Wilderness Attention Conference. Interim Technical Report: 331.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Norman D.A., Shallice T. (1986). Attention to Action. In: Davidson R.J., Schwartz G.E., Shapiro D. (Hrsg.) Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Springer, Boston, MA: 118.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

O’Brien, L. (2007). Self-knowing Agents. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

O’Shaughnessy, B. (1973). Trying. Journal of Philosophy, 70 (13): 365386.

O’Shaughnessy, B. (1980). The Will: A Dual Aspect Theory, Zwei Bände. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ouwehand, C., De Ridder, D.T.D. (2008). Effects of temptation and weight on hedonics and motivation to eat in women. Obesity, 16: 17881793.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ouwehand, C., Papies, K.P. (2010). Eat it or beat it: the differential effect of food temptations on overweight and normal-weight restrained eaters. Appetite, 55: 5660.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pacherie, E. (2000). The content of intentions. Mind and Language, 15 (4): 400432.

Pavlov, I. P (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. An Investigation of the Psychological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Übersetzt und heraugegeben von G.V. Anrep. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pavlov, I.P (1928). Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes. Twenty-five Years of Objective Study of the Higher Nervous Activity (Behaviour) of Animals. Volume 1, translaübersetzt und editiert von W. H. Gantt. New York: International Publishers.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Peake, P.K., Hebl, M., Mischel, W. (2002). Strategic attention deployment in waiting and working situations. Developmental Psychology, 38: 313326.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pearson, K.G., Gordon, J.E. (2013). Spinal Reflexes. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, T.M. Jessell, S.A. Siegelbaum und A.J. Hudespeth (Hrsg.) Principles of Neural Science. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies: 790811.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between cognition and emotion during repsonse inhibition. Emotion, 12 (1), 192197.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pickard, H. (2015). Psychopathology and the Ability to Do Otherwise. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 90 (1): 135163.

Pink, T. (1996). The Psychology of Freedom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Pink, T. (2009). Power and moral responsibility. Philosophical Explorations, 12 (2): 127149.

Pink, T. (2016). Self-Determination: The Ethics of Action, Volume 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pink, T., Stone, M. (Hrsg.) (2004). The Will and Human Action: From Antiquity to the Present Day. Abingdon UK: Routledge.

Plato (1925). Plato in Twelve Volumes, Vol. 9 übersetzt von H.N. Fowler. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.

Prinz, W. (1990). A common-coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann und W. Prinz (Hrsg.). Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches. Berlin, New York: Springer: 167201.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2): 129154.

Prinz, W. (2003). Experimental Approaches to Action In J. Roessler und N. Eilan (Hrsg.). Agency and Self-Awareness. Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 165187.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Pollard, B. (2008). Habits in Action. Saarbrücken: Verlag Dr Müller.

Pollard, B. (2010). Habitual Actions. In T. O’Connor und C. Sandis (Hrsg.): A Companion to the Philosophy of Action. Hoboken NJ: Wiley-Blackwell: 7481.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Rachlin, H. (2001). The Science of Self-Control. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Railton, P. (2009). Practical competence and fluent agency. In D. Sobel und S. Wall (Hrsg.). Reasons for Action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 81115.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Reason, J. (1984). Lapses of attention in everyday life. In R. Parasuraman, D. Davies (Hrsg.) Varieties of Attention. New York: Academic Press: 515549.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Reason. J. (1990). Human Error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ridderinkhof, K.R., Forstmann, B.U., Wylie, S.A., Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W.P.M. (2011). Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: Resisting the call of the Sirens. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2: 174192.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ridderinkhof K.R., van den Wildenberg W.P.M., Brass M. (2014). »Don’t« versus »Won’t«: Principles, Mechanisms, and Intention in Action Inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65: 255262.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Riddoch, M.J., Humphreys, G.W., Edwards, S., Baker, T., Willson, K. (2003). Seeing the action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nature Neuroscience, 6: 8289.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Rietveld, E. (2012). Bodily intentionality and social affordances in context. F. Paglieri (Hrsg.) Consciousness in Interaction. The role of the natural and social context in shaping consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company: 207226.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Rietveld, E., De Haan, S., Denys, D. (2013). Social affordances in context: What is it that we are bodily responsive to? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 36(4): 436.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Rizzolatti, G., Kalaska, J.F. (2013). Voluntary Movements: The Parietal and Premotor Cortex. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, T.M. Jessell, S.A. Siegelbaum und A.J. Hudespeth (Hrsg.) Principles of Neural Science. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies: 865893.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Roelofs, K., Hagenaars, M.A., Stins, J. (2010). Facing freeze: Social threat induces bodily freeze in humans. Psychological Science, 21 (11): 15751581.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Rosvold H.E., Mirsky A.F., Sarason L., Bransome E.D., Beck L.H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20 (5): 343350.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson & Co.

Sanders, A.F. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. In G.E. Stelmach und J. Requin (Hrsg.). Tutorials in Motor Behaviour. Amsterdam: Elsevier: 331-354.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Schlosser, M. (2015). »Agency«, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2015 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (Hrsg.), URL = <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2015/entries/agency/>. (letzter Aufruf 20. August 2018)

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Schmidt, N.B., Richey, J.A., Zvolensky, M.J., Maner, J.K. (2008). Exploring human freeze responses to a threat stressor. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(3): 292304.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Schneider, W., Shiffrin R.M. (1977). Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search and Attention. Psychological Review, 84 (1): 166.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Schroeder, T. (2017). »Desire«, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2017 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (Hrsg.), URL = <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/desire/>. (letzter Aufruf 28. Mai 2018)

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Schultze-Kraft, M., Birman, D., Rusconi, M., Allefeld, C., Görgen, K., Dähne, S., Blankertz, B., Haynes J.D. (2016). The point of no return in vetoing self-initiated movements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 113 (4): 10801085.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Searle, J. (2001). Rationality in Action. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Sechenov, I. (1863/1965). Reflexes of the Brain. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Sel, A., Shepherd, J. (2020). Inhibitory control and self-control. In: A.R. Mele (Hrsg.): Surrounding Self-Control. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 101115.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Sethi, A., Mischel, W., Aber, J.L., Shoda, Y., Rodriguez, M.L. (2000). The role of strategic attention deployment in development of self-regulation: Predicting preschoolers’ delay of gratification from mother–toddler interactions. Developmental Psychology, 36(6): 767777.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Setiya, K. (2007). Reasons Without Rationalism. Princeton N.J.: Princeton University Press.

Shallice, T., Burgess, P.W., Schon, F., Baxter, D.M. (1989). The Origins of Utilization Behavior. Brain, 112: 15871598.

Shepherd, J. (2013). The apparent illusion of conscious deciding. Philosophical Explorations, 16 (1): 1830.

Shepherd, J. (2015). Deciding as Intentional Action: Control over Decisions, Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 93(2): 335351.

Shepherd, J. (2019). Skilled Action and the Double Life of Intention. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 98(2): 286305.

Sherrington, C.S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. 6th edition. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Sherrington, C.S. (1932). Inhibition as Coordinative Factor - Noble Lecture, December 12, 1932. In: Nobel Lectures, Including Presentation Speeches and Laureates’ Biographies. Physiology or Medicine 1922-1941. Amsterdam: Elsevier: 278289.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Sherrington, C.S. (1933). The Brain and its Mechanism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sherrington, C.S. (1940). Man on his Nature. 2nd edition. London: Cambridge University Press.

Siegelbaum, S.A., Kandel E.R., Südhof T.C. (2013). Transmitter Release. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, T.M. Jessell, S.A. Siegelbaum und A.J. Hudespeth (Hrsg.) Principles of Neural Science. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies: 260288.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Sloman, S.A. (1996). The Empirical Case for Two Systems of Reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119 (1): 322.

Smith, R. (1992). History and Meaning in the Sciences of the Mind and Brain. Berkeley CA: University of California Press.

Srull, T.K., Wyer, R.S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about others. Some determinants and implications. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 37: 16601672.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Sripada, C.S. (2010). Philosophical Questions about the Nature of Willpower. Philosophy Compass, 5 (9): 793805.

Sripada, C.S. (2014). How is Willpower Possible? The Puzzle of Synchronic Self‐ Control and the Divided Mind. Noûs, 48 (1): 4174.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Sriapda, C.S. (2018). Addiction and Fallibility. The Journal of Philosophy, 115(11): 569587.

Sripada, C.S. (2020). Atoms of Self-Control. Noûs. Advance online publication: https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12332

Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologia, 30: 276315.

Steward, H. (2009). Sub-intentional actions and the over-mentalization of agency. In C. Sandis (Hrsg.), New Essays on the Explanation of Action. London: Palgrave- Macmillan: 295312.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Steward, H. (2012). A Metaphysics for Freedom. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Steward, H. (2020). Agency as a Two-Way Power: A Defence. The Monist, 103 (3): 342355.

Stice, E., Lawrence, N.S., Kemps, E., Veling, H. (2016). Training motor responses to food: A novel treatment for obesity targeting implicit processes. Clinical Psychology Review, 49: 1627.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Strack, F., Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8 (3): 220247.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18 (6): 643662.

Stroud, S. (2014). Weakness of Will, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2014 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (Hrsg.), URL=<https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2014/entries/weakness-will/>. (letzter Aufruf 28. Mai 2018)

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Teichmann, R. (2010). Anscombe In T. O’Connor und C. Sandis (Hrsg.): A Companion to the Philosophy of Action. London: Wiley-Blackwell: 606612.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Thompson, M. (2008). Life and Action: Elementary Structures of Practice and Practical Thought. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Tucker, M., Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24 (3): 830846.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Tucker, M., Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologia, 116: 185203.

Valenti, S.S., Good, J.M.M. (1991). Social Affordances and Interaction I: Introduction. Ecological Psychology, 3(2): 7798.

Veling, H., Aarts, H., Stroebe, W. (2013). Using stop signals to reduce impulsive choices for palatable unhealthy foods. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 354368.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Velleman, D.J. (1989). Practical Reflections. Princeton N.J.: Princeton University Press.

Velleman, D.J. (1992). What Happens When Someone Acts? Mind, 101 (403): 461481.

Verbruggen, F., De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 21 (2): 391403.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Verbruggen F., Logan, G.D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 137 (4): 649672.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Vogler, C.A. (2002). Reasonably Vicious. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Vohs, K., Heatherton, T. (2000). Self-Regulatory Failure: A Resource-Depletion Approach. Psychological Science, 11 (3): 249254.

Wallace, R.J. (1999). Addiction as defect of the will: Some philosophical reflections. Law and Philosophy, 18(6): 621654.

Watson, G. (1975). Free Agency. Journal of Philosophy, 72: 205220.

Watzl, S. (2017). Structuring Mind. The Nature of Attention and how it Shapes Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Wilson, G., Shpall, S. (2016). »Action«, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Edward N. Zalta (Hrsg.), URL = <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/action/>. (letzter Aufruf 28. Mai 2018)

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophische Untersuchungen. Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp Verlag.

Wollheim, R. (1971). Freud. London: Fontana Press.

Wong, H.Y. (2014). Personal and Sub-Personal: Overcoming Explanatory Apartheid. In T.W. Hung (Hrsg.) Communicative Action. Singapore: Springer Press: 93104.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Wong, H.Y. (2018). Embodied Agency. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97 (3): 584612.

Wu, W. (2008). Visual Attention, Conceptual Content, and Doing it Right. Mind, 117 (468): 10031033.

Wu, W. (2011a). Confronting Many-Many Problems: Attention and Agentive Control. Noûs, 45 (1): 5076.

Wu, W. (2011b). What is Conscious Attention? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 82 (1): 93120.

Wu, W. (2011c). Attention as Selection for Action. In C. Mole, D. Smithies und W. Wu (Hrsg.). Attention: Philosophical and Psychological Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 97116.

  • Search Google Scholar
  • Export Citation

Wu, W. (2014). Attention. Abingdon UK: Routledge.

Wu, W. (2016). Experts and Deviants: The Story of Agentive Control. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 93 (1): 101126.